Assignment: Cognitive Psychology And Its Implications.

24/7 Homework Help

Stuck on a homework question? Our verified tutors can answer all questions, from basic math to advanced rocket science!

Assignment: Cognitive Psychology And Its Implications.

Assignment: Cognitive Psychology And Its Implications.

Respond in 500 words with some scholarly references. Use citations, cite your references. Please use attachment to answer question. Cite every sentence with content from your sources. There are a few ways to do that including just putting the citation at the end of each sentence.  What did you find most interesting or “surprising” about Chapter 7?

  • attachmentchp_7.docx

7

Human Memory:

Retention and Retrieval

Popular fiction frequently has some protagonist who is unable to recall some critical

memory—either because of some head injury, repression of some traumatic

experience, or just because the passage of time has seemed to erase the memory.

The critical turning event in the story occurs when the protagonist is able to recover

the memory—perhaps because of hypnosis, clinical treatment, returning to an old

context, or (particularly improbable) being hit on the head again. Although our everyday

struggles with our memory are seldom so dramatic, we all have had experiences

with memories that are just at the edge of availability. For instance, try remembering

the name of someone who sat beside you in class in grade school or a teacher of a

class. Many of us can picture the person but will experience a real struggle with retrieving

that person’s name—a struggle at which we may or may not succeed. This chapter

will answer the following questions: • How does memory for information fade with the passage of time? • How do other memories interfere with the retrieval of a desired memory? • How can other memories support the retrieval of a desired memory? • How does a person’s internal and external context influence the recall

of a memory? • How can our past experiences influence our behavior without our being able

to recall these experiences? Assignment: Cognitive Psychology And Its Implications.

ORDER NOW FOR CUSTOMIZED, PLAGIARISM-FREE PAPERS

•Are Memories Really Forgotten? Assignment: Cognitive Psychology And Its Implications.

Figure 7.1 repeats Figure 6.1, identifying the prefrontal and temporal structures

that have proved important in studies of memory. This chapter will focus more

on the temporal (and particularly the hippocampal) contributions to memory,

Anderson7e_Chapter_07.qxd 8/20/09 9:47 AM Page 176

Are Memories Really Forgotten? | 177

which play a major role in retention of memory. One of the earliest studies of

the role of the temporal cortex in memory seemed to provide evidence that

forgotten memories are still there even though we cannot retrieve them. As part

of a neurosurgical procedure, Penfield (1959) electrically stimulated portions of

patients’ brains and asked them to report what they experienced (patients were

conscious during the surgery, but the stimulation technique was painless). In

this way, Penfield determined the functions of various portions of the brain.

Stimulation of the temporal lobes led to reports of memories that patients

were unable to report in normal recall, such as events from childhood. This

seemed to provide evidence that much of what seems forgotten is still stored

in memory. Unfortunately, it is hard to know whether the patients’ memory

reports were accurate because there is no way to verify whether the reported

events actually occurred. Therefore, although suggestive, the Penfield experiments

are generally discounted by memory researchers.

A better experiment, conducted by Nelson (1971), also indicates that forgotten

memories still exist. He had participants learn a list of 20 paired associates,

each consisting of a number for which the participant had to recall a

noun (e.g. 43-dog). The subjects studied the list and were tested on it until

they could recall all the items without error. Participants returned for a retest

2 weeks later and were able to recall 75% percent of the associated nouns when

cued with the numbers. However, interest focused on the 25% that they could

no longer recall—were these items really forgotten? Participants were given

new learning trials on the 20 paired associates. The paired associates they had

missed were either kept the same or changed. For example, if a participant had

learned 43-dog but failed to recall the response dog to 43, he or she might now

be trained on either 43-dog (unchanged) or 43-house (changed). Participants

were tested after studying the new list once. If the participants had lost all

memory for the forgotten pairs, there should have been no difference between

recall of changed and unchanged pairs. However, participants correctly recalled

78% of the unchanged items formerly missed, but only 43% of the changed items.

FIGURE 7.1 The brain structures

involved in the creation and

storage of memories. Prefrontal

regions are responsible for

the creation of memories. The

hippocampus and surrounding

structures in the temporal

cortex are responsible for the

permanent storage of these

memories.

Brain Structures

Prefrontal regions

active when information

is retrieved

Hippocampal regions

(internal) active during

retrieval

Anderson7e_Chapter_07.qxd 8/20/09 9:47 AM Page 177

This large advantage for unchanged items indicates that participants had retained

some memory of the original paired associates, even though they had been unable

to recall them initially. Assignment: Cognitive Psychology And Its Implications.

Sometimes we can recognize things we cannot recall. So, Nelson (1978) also

conducted a similar test involving recognition rather than recall. Four weeks

after the initial learning phase, participants failed to recognize 31% of the

paired associates they had learned. As in the previous experiment, Nelson asked

participants to relearn the missed items. For half the stimuli, the responses were

changed; for the other half, they were left unchanged. After one relearning trial,

participants recognized 34% of the unchanged items but only 19% of the

changed items. Thus, even when participants failed this sensitive recognition

test, however, it appears that a record of the item was still in memory—the

evidence again being that relearning was better for the unchanged pairs than

for the changed ones.

These experiments do not prove that everything is remembered. They show

only that appropriately sensitive tests can find evidence for remnants of some

memories that appear to have been forgotten. In this chapter, we will discuss

first how memories become less available with time, then some of the factors

that determine our success in retrieving these memories.

Even when people appear to have forgotten memories, there is evidence that

they still have some of these memories stored.

•The Retention Function

The processes by which memories become less available are extremely regular,

and psychologists have studied their mathematical form. Wickelgren did some

of the most systematic research on memory retention functions, and his data

are still used today. In one recognition experiment (Wickelgren, 1975), he presented

participants with a sequence of words to study and then examined the

probability of their recognizing the words after delays ranging from 1 min to

14 days. Figure 7.2 shows performance as a function of delay. The performance

measure Wickelgren used is called (pronounced d-prime), which is derived

from the probability of recognition. Wickelgren interpreted it as a measure of

memory strength.

Figure 7.2 shows that this measure of memory systematically deteriorates

with delay. However, the memory loss is negatively accelerated—that is, the rate

of change gets smaller and smaller as the delay increases. Figure 7.2b replots

the data as the logarithm of the performance measure versus the logarithm

of delay. Marvelously, the function becomes linear. The log of performance is

a linear function of the log of the delay T; that is,

where is the value of the function at 1 min [log(1) = 0] and is the slope

of the function in Figure 7.2b, which happens to be 0.30 in this case.

log d¿ = A – b log T

¿

178 | Human Memory: Retention and Retrieval

Anderson7e_Chapter_07.qxd 8/20/09 9:47 AM Page 178

This equation can be transformed to

where _ 10and is 3.62 in this case. That is, these performance measures are

power functions of delay. In a review of research on forgetting, Wixted and

Ebbesen (1991) concluded that retention functions are generally power functions.

This relationship is called the power law of forgetting. Recall from Chapter

6 that there is also a power law of learning: Practice curves are described by

power functions. Both functions are negatively accelerated, but with an important

difference. Whereas practice functions show diminishing improvement

with practice, retention functions show diminishing loss with delay.

A very dramatic example of the negative acceleration in retention function

was produced by Bahrick (1984), who looked at participants’ retention of English-

Spanish vocabulary items anywhere from immediately to 50 years after they

had completed courses in high school and college. Figure 7.3 plots the number

of items correctly recalled out of a total of 15 items as a function of the logarithm

of the time since course completion. Separate functions are plotted for

students who had one, three, or five courses. The data show a slow decay of

knowledge combined with a substantial practice effect. In Bahrick’s data, the

retention functions are nearly flat between 3 and 25 years (as would be predicted

by a power function), with some further drop-off from 25 to 49 years (which is

more rapid than would be predicted by a power function). Bahrick (personal

communication, circa 1993) suspects that this final drop-off is probably related

to physiological deterioration in old age.

There is some evidence that the explanation for these decay functions may

be found in the associated neural processes. Recall from Chapter 6 that longterm

potentiation (LTP) is an increase in neural responsiveness that occurs as a

reaction to prior electrical stimulation.We saw that LTP mirrors the power law

d¿ = cT-b

The Retention Function | 179

(a) (b)

0

1.0

2.0

5

Delay, (days)

3.62−0.321

Measure of retention (ʹ)

10 15 20 1

.1

2

.2

.5

1.0

2.0

3.0

4 7 15 30 50 1 2 4 71014

Minutes

log ʹ

Days

log T

Hire a competent writer to help you with

Assignment: Cognitive Psychology And Its Implications.

troublesome homework